The Evolutionary Perspective: Genetic Inheritance from our Ancestors
Learning Outcomes
- Describe the evolutionary perspective
The fundamentals of the evolutionary perspective
Evolutionary psychology has its historical roots in Charles Darwin’s theory of natural selection. In The Origin of Species, Darwin predicted that psychology would develop an evolutionary basis, and that a process of natural selection creates traits in a species that are adaptive to its environment.
Using Darwin’s arguments, evolutionary approaches claim that one’s genetic inheritance not only determine such physical traits as skin and eye color, but also certain personality traits and social behaviors. For example, some evolutionary developmental psychologists suggest that behavior such as shyness and jealousy may be produced in part by genetic causes, presumably because they helped increase the survival rates of human’s ancient relatives.[1][2][3]
Lorenz and Imprinting
The evolutionary perspective draws heavily on the field of ethology, which examines the ways in which our biological makeup influences our behavior. The primary proponent of ethology was Konrad Lorenz, who discovered that newborn geese are genetically pre-programmed to become attached to the first moving object they see after birth. Lorenz’s work led developmentalists to consider the ways in which human behavior might reflect inborn genetic patterns. Working with geese, he investigated the principle of imprinting, the process by which some nidifugous birds (i.e. birds that leave their nest early) bond instinctively with the first moving object that they see within the first hours of hatching. Although Lorenz did not discover the topic, he became widely known for his descriptions of imprinting as an instinctive bond.
In psychology and ethology, imprinting is any kind of phase-sensitive learning (learning occurring at a particular age or a particular life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior. It was first used to describe situations in which an animal or person learns the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be “imprinted” onto the subject. Imprinting is hypothesized to have a critical period.
Behavioral Genetics
The evolutionary perspective encompasses one of the fastest-growing areas within the field of lifespan development: behavioral genetics. Behavioral genetics is a field of scientific research that uses genetic methods to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behavior and studies the effects of heredity on behavior. Behavioral geneticists strive to understand how we might inherit certain behavioral traits and how the environment influences whether we actually displayed those traits. It also considers how genetic factors may influence psychological disorders such as schizophrenia, depression and substance abuse.[4][5][6]
Link to Learning
In Stanford, professor and author of Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers, Robert Sapolsky’s Ted Talk, Sapolsky describes how our history and biology influence our behavior. This tour of our individual and collective history provides an enlightening overview of behavioral genetics.
Criticisms of the Evolutionary Perspective
Try It
https://assess.lumenlearning.com/practice/1a38a672-43b9-48fa-990a-26f802ce26de
https://assess.lumenlearning.com/practice/b30548d9-2f47-43e6-a975-c37d5e7e2375
Glossary
[glossary-page]
[glossary-term]behavioral genetics:[/glossary-term]
[glossary-definition]one of the fastest-growing areas within the field of lifespan development and studies the effects of heredity on behavior[/glossary-definition]
[glossary-term]ethology:[/glossary-term]
[glossary-definition]the study of behavior through a biological lens[/glossary-definition]
[glossary-term]evolutionary psychology:[/glossary-term]
[glossary-definition]a field of study that seeks to identify behavior that is a result of our genetic inheritance from our ancestors[/glossary-definition]
[glossary-term]imprinting:[/glossary-term]
[glossary-definition]in psychology and ethology, imprinting is any kind of phase-sensitive learning (learning occurring at a particular age or a particular life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior[/glossary-definition]
[/glossary-page]
Contribute!
- David M. Buss The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating BasicBooks, Jun 25, 2003 ↵
- Buss, A.H 2012 Pathways to individuality: evolution and development of personality traits. Washington, DC: American psychological Association ↵
- Easton, JM., Schipper, L., And Shackleford, T. 2007 morbid jealousy from an evolutionary psychological perspective. Evolution and human behavior, 28, 399 –402 ↵
- Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, Bruce J. Ellis, David F. Bjorklund pp 3-18 New York Guilford Press, Jan 1, 2005 ↵
- Rembis , M. 2009( re)defining disability in the “genetic age”: behavioral genetics, “new” eugenics and the future of impairment. Disability and society, 24, 585 - 597 ↵
- PLOMIN, R., DEFRIES, J. C. , KnOPIK, V. S., & NEIDERHISER, J. M. 2016. Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspectives on psychological science, 11, 3–23. ↵
- Bjorklund, D. 2006 mother knows best, epigenetic inheritance, maternal effects, and the evolution of human intelligence. Developmental review, 26, 213 –242. ↵
- Baptista, T., Aldana, E., Angeles , F., And Beaulieu , S. 2008. Evolution theory: an overview of its applications in psychiatry. Psychopathology, 41, 17 –27. ↵
- Del Giudice, M. 2015. Self-regulation in an evolutionary perspective. In G. E. Gendolla, M. Tops, S. L. Koole, G. E. Gendolla, M. Tops, & S. L. Koole (Eds), Handbook of behavioral approaches to self-regulation. New York, New York: Springer science + business media ↵